Musings with a Manitoba ECE

Category: Multimedia Learning

Reflections

Photo by Marc-Olivier Jodoin on Unsplash

The thing I like most about education is that it is a process of personal evolution. It can be easy to settle into patterns in your career and life, but education introduces turbulence to a stagnant pool. Each new thought or bit of information that flows into the pool disrupts the surface a little bit. The reflection of myself that I see now is necessarily different from what I saw at the beginning of this course. I have new ripples on the surface that give me a different perspective and hint at the depth of my learning.

Interactive and Multimedia Learning Theories has been an interesting class and initiated a thought-provoking journey into my inquiry topic. It also provided background information and skills that will enable me to become a more efficient researcher and effective practitioner. Indeed, the creation of this blog has facilitated not just the dissemination of my learning, but enabled me to track and explore it, as well as relate it to my practice. In each post rather than just dumping a bunch of academic research, I was able to reflect on the content and my experience. This has helped keep my learning relevant since it is so easy to slide down interesting academic rabbit-holes.

Early on in the course I was introduced to the concept of digital literacy which, combined with media literacy, are critical skill-sets for the connected citizen. By being aware of them, I am able to be a more responsible and critical consumer (and creator) of content. Since I am an educator, I need to be aware of how my use of digital technologies affects and influences others as well as myself.

As this was my first online course, and the content was fairly self-directed, I was a bit apprehensive about my ability to stay on task and not get lost in my own little research world. The regular online face-to-face meetings and my learning pod made me feel like part of a community which greatly alleviated my fears and kept me connected to my peers. In fact my learning pod was so helpful that my colleagues at Entangled Curiosities, Littles in the Forrest, and Children’s Stories and I decided to keep meeting online for the duration of the program, if not longer, to break down our learning and to network.

As far as writing goes, I have always felt that it was my strong suit. However, I have relied a fair bit on my printer and my memory in compiling research papers throughout my academic career. Zotero though, is a game-changer. I tried out this citation manager for my final paper this term and it was quite a different process. I didn’t print a single article, I didn’t laboriously type out the entire reference list, and it was amazing! I will definitely use it throughout this program and beyond. I also have already started using it to organize articles that I use in my practice or that I may find useful later on. Being able to find the article I am looking for and extract the juicy bits is a whole new level of organization for me.

The tools, networks, and skills I learned greatly facilitated my research. By drawing from my practice with infants, conversing with my learning pod, and engaging with the blogs of my colleagues, I eventually settled on a research topic: how infants are socialized to understand mobile devices. I had started digging into the literature on infant development and digital technologies, but it didn’t quite feel like a juicy enough topic. Besides, there was a fair bit of research that said similar things: infants aren’t developmentally able to benefit from screens, and they need face-to-face interaction to learn. These articles also talked about how children’s screen/tech use tended to mirror their parents’ use. Then I found a link in laucoo’s blog that referenced role modelling tech etiquette. This was the smoking gun. I could tie together infant’s innate ability to learn from others, role modelling, and adult’s use of tech. By marrying these topics, I was able to examine how infants are socialized to understand mobile device use and make recommendations for parents and practitioners. Basically, we as adults need to be cognizant of our influence on tiny minds and exhibit desirable tech etiquette, as well as ensure that our devices don’t displace valuable face-to-face connections with our little charges.

The feedback I received for my paper was that it was fascinating, if a bit scary. The idea that adults are socializing infants to understand that mobile devices are often more important to us than interacting with them is not a comfortable thought. One thing I mentioned, but could have devoted more time to, was that mindfulness and moderation are key, and nobody is trying to be a bad parent or role model. This paper wasn’t intended to judge anyone, but to highlight some information so that we can tweak our behaviour to better meet our desired outcomes: well-adjusted babies.

So when I look into the pool now, I see myself differently. I see someone who recognizes the importance of mindfully using modern technologies. I am more cognizant of how the little things I do around infants can contribute to their growing understanding of how people operate in the world, whether it is my intention or not. I am a more efficient researcher, and more mindful of my connections and networks with other practitioners. We can keep disrupting each others’ pools for a long time to come.

How do mobile devices impact face-to-face interactions?

Photo by Wyron A on Unsplash

The other day as I was leaving work, I observed an interaction where a father was helping his two-year-old son get dressed into his snowsuit to go home, all the while talking animatedly on his cell phone. The father was paying attention to the child enough to functionally get him ready to go, but there didn’t seem to be any deeper attention paid, or conversation between the two. I feel like these kinds of interactions are relatively common, and perhaps I only really noticed because I have been thinking about tech use within the infant/adult dyad. This incident made me want to dig deeper into last week’s topic and examine how mobile devices impact face-to-face interactions. Kildare’s and Middlemiss’ literature review (2017), Impact of Parents’ Mobile Device use on Parent-Child Interaction, dug in to describe how the quality of parent/child interactions is affected by parental device use. The iPhone Effect: The Quality of In-Person Social Interactions in the Presence of Mobile Devices by Misra, Cheng, Genevie (2016), and Yuan looked at how the presence (or absence) of a mobile device, whether in use or not, affected in-person conversation partners perceived attention, closeness, and empathy during conversation.

Having already discussed the ubiquity of mobile devices in children’s and parents’ lives, as well as the pressure parents’ feel to stay connected, we’ll get straight to the meat of the articles.

The iPhone effect: The quality of in-person social interactions in the presence of mobile devices

“One can communicate with a social group or an individual, regardless of proximity or location, thereby elevating a spatially distant relationship over proximal, face-to-face relationships (Gergen, 2002).” (Misra et al., 2016, p. 280)

Misra and colleagues commented on the sensory overload people experience living primarily urban lives, and how people retreat from this overload by selectively tuning out – like listening to podcasts on public transit rather than engaging with other passengers. They say that there is a new form of cognitive overload, which they call cyber-based overload, which “taxes individuals’ working memory, amplif[ies] distractedness, and mak[es] it difficult for them to distinguish between relevant and irrelevant information”.

They also go on to say that phones are not just phones, but a symbolic representation of people\s wider social networks and all the information and communication that phones connect us to. As some of my colleagues would say, phones have agency and purpose, and so their mere presence fundamentally alters an interaction. Misra et al. conducted a naturalistic experiment where they studied how a person’s smart phone or tablet just being placed on the table where they were conversing with another person affected the quality of their face to face conversation. It was an elegantly designed study with interesting results:

  • “If either participant placed a mobile communication device (e.g., smartphone or a cell phone) on the table or held it in their hand during the course of the 10-min conversation, the quality of the conversation was rated to be less fulfilling compared with conversations that took place in the absence of mobile devices.” (Misra et al., 2016, p. 290)
  • “participants who conversed in the presence of mobile communication devices also reported experiencing lower empathetic concern compared with participants who interacted without distracting digital stimuli in their visual field.” (Misra et al,  2016, p. 290)
  • “The relationship between the presence of mobile devices and empathetic concern was more pronounced for participants who reported a closer relationship with each other compared with those who were less familiar with each other.” (Misra et al., 2016, p. 290)

Misra et al. suggested that the reason for this is that while conversation partners were aware of the presence and potential of the mobile device, they were “potentially more likely to miss subtle cues, facial expressions, and changes in the tone of their conversation partner’s voice, and have less eye contact”. All these verbal and non-verbal cues are important to a fulfilling conversation.

So, the takeaway from this article is that phones are distracting, even when not in use. This distraction represents a cognitive overload which lowers the quality of face-to-face interactions. This study was with adults, but Kildare and Middlemiss studied the impact of device use on parent-child interactions.

Impact of parents’ mobile device use on parent-child interaction: A literature review

“Parents who use their phones during parent-child interactions are less sensitive and responsive both verbally and nonverbally to their children’s bids for attention, potentially leading to lower quality parent-child interactions.” (Kildare & Middlemiss, 2017, p. 579)

Kildare and Middlemiss begin by discussing the displacement hypothesis which I was not familiar with by name but recognize as a common discourse in the nature vs. tech dialogue. Basically, it posits that “time spent with & technology or media may displace and decrease meaningful parent-child connections”. This is critical because it is well-established that child development is strongly influenced by parents’ behaviour and parent-child interactions.

This hypothesis echoes the findings of Misra et al. when they describe the way one retreats from cognitive or sensory overload by selectively focusing. Both articles discuss the way that mobile devices take precedence over face-to-face communication due to the representational nature of the tech. Kildare and Middlemiss also comment on the fact that people are expected to be available to respond to their devices all the time, and the pressure to be continually connected interferes with quality face-to-face interactions. Here are selection of the conclusions that I found the most interesting:

  • 35% of American adults reported to frequently using their phone while playing with their children
  • Parents often use their smart phones to stay connected with their teens, but use it to deliberately disconnect from their younger children by using social media to connect with other adults instead
  • 52% of parents reported checking their phone too frequently, and 28% felt that they were not modeling appropriate device use

The authors also commented on the fact that as parents’ devise use potentially displaces parent-child interactions, parents may experience less positive parenting moments. This, especially where young children are involved, has the potential to cause parents to retreat further into their phone to escape the negative interactions.

Toward the end of the article Kildare and Middlemiss discuss parents’ role modeling of mobile devices, which, to me, is a critically important factor. Young children learn about mobile devices and the social norms surrounding them through observing others interacting with them. So not only are young children’s potential for quality interactions being displaced, but they are learning that the mobile devices is more important than them. The authors wrap up the article by suggesting that parents limit their use of mobile tech in the presence of their children, however even they recognize that this is no easy task.

 

Parents’ Use of, and Communication around, Digital and Mobile Devices

Photo by Giang Vu on Unsplash

“Parents shape their children’s media habits from the time of infancy, through setting limits on amount and content of media use, helping children understand what they encounter on screens, and role modeling technology use.” (Redesky et al., 2016, p. 694)

An important facet of my inquiry is determining how adults and young children (particularly infants) actually communicate in an environment where digital and/or mobile technologies are being used. In A Naturalistic Study of Child and Family Screen Media and Mobile Device Use by Domoff et al. the authors studied what was being said by, and to, young children when any screen or mobile device was in use in their immediate home environment. They had several interesting findings:

  • Parents’ mediation of screen media tended to be reactive and aimed at restricting use or explaining technology functionality.
  • Active mediation was initiated most by the children (in preschool and school-age groups) asking or commenting on the content of the media. This was less evident in the infant group as they lacked the speaking skills to initiate those dialogues.
  • Siblings played a more dominant role in mediation than parents.
  • Parents and children negotiated screen time limits.
  • “Parallel family media use was common. Multiple family members engaged with their own mobile devices while simultaneously being exposed to background screen media (i.e., media multitasking).” (Domoff et al., 2019, p. 401)
  • “Active mediation of TV has been found to mitigate the risks of exposure to violent media (e.g., aggression; Nathanson and Cantor 2000), and enhance positive effects of prosocial media (e.g., Daniel Tiger’s Neighborhood; Rasmussen et al. 2016).” (Domoff et al., 2019, p. 402)

The second point was the most interesting to me as my research to date has highlighted the importance of mediating screen media with very young children. Domoff et al. describe active mediation as “consist[ing] of parents’ communication with their children about media content, including characters’ actions and motivations”. It looks then like this is not happening very much with the youngest children. If parents don’t often initiate active mediation, then the ones with more consistent speech skills are privileged, as they are able to initiate these interactions. The authors last point about family members engaging with separate devices while together made me wonder about whether the adults were so wrapped up in their own device use, that their interactions with their children were reactive rather than proactive.

Then I wanted to know about parents’ perceptions of their own media use around their children. Enter Parent Perspectives on Their Mobile Technology Use : The Excitement and Exhaustion of Parenting While Connected by Radesky et al.. In it, the authors describe a study where they conducted a series of interviews to elicit parents’ nuanced and complex feelings toward they way that they use screen and mobile technology around their infants.

“Cognitively, they described the act of multitasking between technology and children as stressful or less effective; specifically, they described how the cognitive load of doing work or accessing information via technology often makes it difficult to read and respond to children’s social cues in the moment” (Redesky et al., 2016, p. 699).

Parents felt tensions between their tech use and their parental and daily responsibilities. This supports the Domoff’s findings that parents interactions around technology use tend to be reactive and rarely focus on the content that their children are accessing unless the child brings it up. It seems that parents are distracted by their own devices and so are less mindful of their children’s experiences with screen and mobile media than they might like to be. The question is, how does parental use of mobile tech influence children’s socialization and development? Does parents’ attention to their devices impact their child’s sense of self worth? What kind of role-modelling are we adults doing? It brings me back to the idea of consciously role-modelling tech etiquette with young children. It seems we also need to be more mindful of actively mediating young children’s tech use, especially if they are too young to ask us about it.

Parents as Gatekeepers

Photo by Dongho Kim on Unsplash

As I was reflecting on technology use within the infant/adult dyad, I remembered an article from a previous class, A threat to childhood innocence or the future of learning? Parents’ perspectives on the use of touch-screen technology by 0–3 year-olds in the UK by O’Connor and Fotakopoulou (2018). In it the authours conducted a study on the relationship between parents’ perspectives and young children’s use of touchscreen technology. I was also drawn to a study recommended by a peer on tech use by a toddler within the social context of her family as a form of multimodal literacy. The article was ‘i’Babies: Infants’ and toddlers’ emergent language and literacy in a digital culture of iDevices by Harrison and McTavish (2018).

In their article, O’Connor and Fotakopoulou raised a seemingly obvious point, that need to be articulated none the less: parents’ views on the appropriateness of tech use for young children determines the degree to which they allow their children to access it. I further posit that parents’ views also determine the degree to which they mediate their children’s tech use.

Harrison and McTavish remind us that as we consider multiliteracies and the skills associated with them, we must also consider that tech use is a literacy as well. However, O’Connor and Fotakopoulou acknowledge that many do not accept touchscreen use (and other tech skills) as a part of “normal development and “reflects a conceptual separation between technological skills and other skills that young children need to develop, as well as a common bias towards more ‘natural’ occupations for very young children.”

Discourses of childhood

O’Connor and Fotakopoulou went on to assert that the there are competing discourses of childhood and depending on how an individual views children and childhood their attitudes toward tech use among children will be either positive or negative.

The first discourse they identified is that childhood is a natural time of innocence, and children need to be protected from the harsh, polluting influence of the adult world. This leads to protectionist behaviour toward children, and people who hold this view tend to protect children’s innocence from the dangers of tech use.

The second discourse recognizes the opportunities that technology offers children. This is a future oriented view where children build important skills that will enable them to effectively navigate the future worlds of education and work.

Parents’ views

“Parents are the main gatekeepers of their young children’s use of technology. It follows, then, that their beliefs and behaviour in relation to parenting, child development and learning are going to be instrumental in the access their 0-3s have to this technology.” (O’Connor & Fotakopoulou, 2016, p. 236)

In their study, O’Connor and Fotakopoulou found that both discourses were represented. Many “parents encouraged and supported their children’s early experiences with digital technologies” for the perceived future benefits. Harrison and McTavish also report that studies find that parents make time for young children to play on tablets, especially with open-ended educational apps.

“This awareness of the need for future technological skills has not been documented before in relation to children as young as this and is an important indicator of the impact touch screens may be having on perceptions around very early childhood education and play.” (O’Connor & Fotakopoulou, 2016, p. 241)

Parents also expressed concerns about the potential dangers of touchscreen use such as accessing inappropriate content and the possible negative impact on social and communication skills. These fears represent thinking within the ‘natural childhood’ discourse, where the innocence of childhood needs to be protected.

Although I am not a parent, I have been thinking about my own views on children and technology and talking to others. In my line of work, I have access to several parents of infants and have been casually collecting viewpoints. The parents I’ve spoken to tended to lean toward a more protectionist view, and heavily limit their children’s use of digital tech, although the second discourse was represented as well.

The seductive nature of touchscreens

A worry expressed by some of the parents I spoke to was the seemingly addictive nature of touchscreens (and other screens). This was also taken up in the literature and Harrison and McTavish described a scene where their toddler subject woke from a nap and immediately sought out her mother’s phone to watch videos before even greeting her mother. O’Connor and Fotakopoulou also reported that parents were concerned about how ‘the iPad version of reality [was] more colourful, easier and more appealing than real life e.g. colouring in an iPad [was] much easier than on real paper with real pens”.

How children use tech

The 23 month-old subject of Harrison’s and McTavish’s study demonstrated sophisticated manipulation of the software on the touchscreen devices she used. She used her index fingers and thumbs to tap, swipe, and expand certain areas. She was able to navigate to her favourite apps and galleries and within YouTube.

Both articles discussed children using touchscreens to FaceTime or video chat with absent family members, and according to O’Connor and Fotakopoulou, this was the least contentious use of touchscreens. Educational apps, and games were also used by participants in both studies.

Although not identified in O’Connor’s and Fotakopoulou’s questionnaire, Harrison and McTavish reported that their study participant’s use of touchscreen technology was heavily mediated by family members.

Parental mediation

“Ally interacts both culturally and socially with her family members through her smartphone and tablet activities. Ally’s family members are constantly mediating her use of symbolic tools (iDevices) simply because an iDevice belongs to that member and they have a vested interest that Ally does not accidently Skype or text someone inadvertently, or delete photos, contacts and apps. Hence, Ally’s interaction on these iDevices is, more often than not, a shared activity, which makes it inherently social.” (Harrison & McTavish, 2018, p. 184)

From all that I have read so far, there seems to be a growing consensus that when (or if) very young children use digital technologies such as touch screens that their use should be mediated by an engaged adult. This is in recognition of the fact that children learn through interaction with more experienced others (Vygotsky’s Sociocultural Theory).

Babies, the adults who care about them, and tech use

Photo by Alexander Dummer on Unsplash

In the article Infants, Toddlers and Learning from Screen Media by Courage & Troseth (2016), the authors conducted a literature review to examine whether or not infants and toddlers can learn from screen media. The evidence they examined indicated that no, infants under the age of 2 do not learn from screen media. They said that “although infants and toddlers are remarkably capable learners in direct social interaction, their language and story comprehension skills are limited and they are unlikely to follow the narrative content, story line, or content to be learned from a video or app”. Starting at about 15 months, with co-viewing and explicit adult participation to mediate the child’s understanding of screen media, children can learn from educational programming. The big caveat according to the authors is that they still don’t learn as much through mediated viewing as the would through regular play and interaction with a caring adult.

These findings aligned with the Canadian Paediatric Society’s position statement, Screen time and young children: Promoting health and development in a digital world, which also argued that there were no substantiated claims that children under the age of 2 received any benefit from screen time. The statement further claimed that “there [was] solid evidence that infants and toddlers [had]difficulty transferring new learning from a 2D representation to a 3D object (e.g., from screen to real life) and [were] unlikely to learn from TV at this age”.

Another aspect of screen media that Courage and Troseth examined was background television use. They claimed that children exposed to background television were less engaged in their play, they directed frequent quick looks at the television, and showed less focused attention on their toys. When they engaged with adults, the adults tended to give delayed and less focused responses when the TV was on.

This made me think about how we as adults role-model screen use, and how children are learning the norms of social interaction with, and around screens. Another student in my cohort posted this blog post by Karen Nelson that, although aimed at pre-K and kindergarten teachers, brought up an intriguing point:  we should be modeling appropriate tech etiquette. In my practice, children often use anything that will fit in their hand as a phone, and walk around ‘talking’ on it, or passing the toy to a teacher to take the call. They have obviously seen people using phones, but in what contexts? We might be play eating in the housekeeping centre when one of the children passes me a phone. Normally I would accept the call and talk briefly on the phone to engage with the child and scaffold their play. But am I just role-modeling poor tech etiquette? By accepting the pretend call, I am indicating that the phone takes precedence over the face to face interactions I am having while I pretend to eat with other children. And I hate when people answer their phones at dinner!

I’m beginning to get some vague ideas about where I want to take this. It has something to do with role-modeling tech use, distraction potential of background screens for both children and adults, and the necessity to mediate young children’s use of digital and screen media. I guess the intersection of tech, infants, and adults and how to optimally navigate these interactions. It seems to all boil down to mindfulness.

A Closer Look – Reading More into Mayer’s 12 Principles for Multimedia Learning

One of my course readings this week was Using multimedia for e-learning by Mayer (2017). In it, the author discussed 12 research-based principles for designing computer-based learning tools. As an ECE practitioner working with infants and toddlers, I initially had trouble relating it to my practice.

I do not use computer-based learning tools in my work, but as a practitioner living in the 21st century, it behooves me to have the skills and knowledge to evaluate tools as they become more and more a part of our lives. The reality is that children have digital technologies available to them at younger and younger ages. These technologies are a part of most children’s and families’ lives where I practice. So, while I do not use these technologies with the children I interact with, I can’t predict the future and so should be able to make research-based decisions about what I include in my practice, as well as what I exclude from it. In the spirit of growth and understanding, I thought I would deepen my comprehension of the 12 principles that Mayer outlines by relating them to a multimedia tool that I am intimately familiar with – the picture book.

Multimedia

  • Mayer describes multimedia as the combination of words and pictures to enhance learning. Words can be either spoken or written, and pictures can be either static or moving (such as animation).
  • In a children’s picture book, both the written text of the story and the illustrations combine to enhance children’s learning. The varying degrees of quality in picture books can, in part, be explained by the following principles.

Reducing Extraneous Processing

Principles 2 through 6 all relate to increasing the efficiency of cognitive processing by reducing extra processing tasks. They are:

Coherence

  • “People learn better when extraneous material is excluded” (Mayer, 2017, Table 1).
  • When an illustrator includes images not related to the words in the illustrations, these act as distractions for children who are trying to process the meaning of the words and pictures. For example, in a book on farm animals, on the page labeled ‘Pig’ it would be distracting to have pictures of other animals alongside the pig.

Signalling

  • “People learn better when essential material is highlighted” (Mayer, 2017, Table 1).
  • In Wild Berries by Julie Flett (2013), the vocabulary words in Swampy Cree n dialect are highlighted by the red colour of the text, indicating that this is important material.

Redundancy

  • “People learn better from graphics and narration than from graphics, narration, and on-screen text” (Mayer, 2017, Table 1).
  • Honestly, I had trouble relating this to picture books, and the best I could come up with was comparing a felt-board story performance (no visible text) to a picture book narration. However, that crosses over into other principles like embodiment described further below. Perhaps there are limitations to using children’s picture books as a metaphor for everything. How disappointing. If you have ideas, I’d love to hear them!

Spatial Contiguity

  • “People learn better when on-screen words are placed next to the corresponding part of the graphic” (Mayer, 2017, Table 1).
  • For example, in a picture of a construction site, having the names of the machines and equipment next to the corresponding image helps to connect the written word to the object it is representing.

Temporal Contiguity

  • “People learn better when corresponding narration and graphics are presented simultaneously” (Mayer, 2017, Table 1).
  • It is easier for children to integrate the illustrations with the story if they can see the pictures as they are hearing the words, rather than having the reader first read the text aloud and then show the picture.

Managing Essential Processing

The following 3 principles are related to managing the cognitive processing of complex or difficult learning. They are:

Segmenting

  • “People learn better when a multimedia lesson is presented in small, user paced segments” (Mayer, 2017, Table 2).
  • For example, infant board books that depict a single word with only the corresponding image on each opening allow the user to absorb the material at their own pace before turning the page.

Pre-training

  • “People learn better when they learn the key terms prior to receiving a multimedia lesson” (Mayer, 2017, Table 2).
  • Through daily practice and through their knowledge of the children, practitioners are able to select picture books that reflect and expand on children’s knowledge and interests rather than introducing new topics without preparation. This is scaffolding as I know it. Building on a foundation of prior knowledge.

Modality

  • “People learn better from a multimedia presentation when the words are presented in spoken form” (Mayer, 2017, Table 2).
  • This is an obvious one for the children I work with since they do not yet read. Even for children who are able to read, if the material is complex, it is easier to understand if it narrated while you look at the illustration than if you are trying to reconcile the text with the illustration. This means that the child is processing the auditory information in conjunction with the visual information, rather than having two separate sources of visual information to reconcile.

Fostering Generative Processing

The following 3 principles discuss research-based methods for encouraging the learner to engage in greater depth with the material presented in a multimedia lesson.

Personalization

  • “People learn better when the words in a multimedia lesson are presented in conversational style rather than formal style” (Mayer, 2017, Table 3).
  • Some picture books draw the reader in by asking questions of the reader or making them part of the story. Books about body parts may talk about ‘your belly-button’ or ‘your toes’. Or they may say something like ‘here’s Teddy’s belly-button. Where is your belly-button?’. This draws children in and it’ more engaging to them than anybody’s old belly-button.

Voice

  • “People learn better from a human voice than from a machine-like voice” (Mayer, 2017, Table 3).
  • There are many battery-operated picture books out there where the child can push a button and here the corresponding sound or word. My experience with these types of books is that they are great for Pavlovian cause and effect learning. The child pushes a button and hears a mechanical voice. They aren’t however increasing the child’s understanding of the material presented in the book. Children seem to get more from books if someone reads the book to them, rather than just pushing a button over and over so that the mechanical voice continuously says ‘Triangle. Triangle. Triangle
”

Embodiment

  • “People learn better when an on-screen agent uses human-like gestures and movement” (Mayer, 2017, Table 3).
  • This point makes me wonder about the popularity of anthropomorphizing animals and inanimate objects in children’s books. Obviously, books are different from screens, but the idea of humanizing non-humans in order to teach concepts has been in practice from the earliest storytelling traditions. I have never liked the idea of vehicles having human-like faces and emotions, but they are ubiquitous in children’s literature (and toys). So, do children learn better with anthropomorphic animals and objects? Is this related to the principle of embodiment, or am I drawing a false correlation?

I am cognizant of the fact that the source article was specifically about e-learning, and by taking the tech out of the principles, I was sort of missing the point. I do feel that this exercise allowed me to engage more deeply with the concepts, even if it was a bit of a stretch. Yes, picture books are a multimedia learning tool, especially if they are narrated, as they frequently are in my practice. No, they are not computer-based. I could have examined all these principles through YouTube narrations of picture books, but since these are not part of my practice, it would have been disingenuous.

Did I learn something? Yes. Was it a bit of a stretch? Yes. Will I use the principles in my practice? Well, I have a new tool-set for choosing and reading books to my babies. That’s worthwhile. Maybe I’ll be able to use these principles for computer-based learning in the future. Who knows what infant practice will look like in 10 years!

Settling into my Inquiry

Photo by Kevin Ku on Unsplash

I have narrowed down my inquiry for this blog (and this course) into an exploration of the effects of digital technology use on young children’s development. I know that there are some strong feelings from both sides: those who see technology as a barrier to healthy child development, and those who feel that by providing children with these tools we are giving them an edge in this technologically driven word. My goal is to sift through the studies and explore the academic and professional literature to come up with an evidence-informed guide for practice. As I work with infants, my focus will be for very young children, but I expect there will be more literature for school-aged children, and this will likely take some sifting.

The task I’ve been set this week was to explore some of the existing projects submitted by UVic students in partial fulfillment of their MEd degrees. I selected Pina Hendry’s project entitled What is the Impact of Digital Technology on Young Students? , which seemed an obvious fit for my inquiry.

Right away I could tell that the author approached the topic with a fair amount of caution. As a kindergarten teacher practicing for over 20 years, she had noted the decline in fine motor skills of entering students, among other changes to things like attention, and language skills. The literature she reviewed seemed to provide evidence to support her opinion that digital technology has detrimental effects on cognitive, social, emotional, and physical development. As a practitioner, I have shared this opinion and abide by the Canadian Paediatric Society’s screen time guidelines for young children (found here). However, as with everything else in life, I find that the more I examine a topic, the more nuanced it proves to be. When one refers to digital technology there are so many possible candidates that lumping them all together under the umbrella of ‘digital technology’ and making sweeping proclamations seems disingenuous. Indeed, the literature reviewed looked at samples as varied as television, violent video games, pornography, and social media. I did not see any reference to the apps, programs, games or sites designed to educate or encourage healthy development, of which even I know there are at least some out in the world.

My impression of the project was that by focusing on the dangers of digital tech use for children, we can really see how dangerous they are. I agree that unfettered and unsupervised access to these technologies can be detrimental to each aspect of development. However, while engaging my inner skeptic, I did not see that the opposing argument was adequately accounted for.

 

In order to balance my view on the topic, I wanted to hear an argument from the other side, and came across the TED talk by Sara DeWitt called 3 Fears About Screen Time for Kids – and Why They’re Not True. Now, my inner skeptic was engaged for this too, and this whole experience is supporting my hypothesis of learning: the more you learn about something, the less able you are to give a definitive answer to a simple question about it.

DeWitt addressed the 3 fears (a. screens are passive; b. playing games on screens is a waste of time; and c. screens isolate parents from their children) and responded to each one with an example. The problem I found was that for each fear, she provided an example of a single app or program that addresses that fear. She did not say that the fears are unfounded, or that they were not true, despite the title of the talk.

So, in reflecting on both the master’s project and the TED talk, I found that neither Hendry nor DeWitt support unfettered, unsupervised access to digital technology for young children. They also both advocate for thoughtful planning and supervision by parents to ensure that the digital tools children are accessing are appropriate, and if not support, at least not hinder healthy development. Despite seeming to come at the issue from opposite sides, Hendry and DeWitt come to the same conclusions: caution is necessary.

Bringing it to Work

“Copeful: Get Through Grief Together” by Megan Daley is licensed under CC BY-NC 4.0

Over the past week I have been making informal inquiries at work about our technology policies and how they are formed. My director informed me that we do have a technology policy, but it was related to staff use of tech, rather than how we implement tech use with the children. In a search of our policy manuals available to the parents, I found no mention of it. I suggested that through the resources available to me through the EDCI 567 class, I may soon be in a position to provided advice on appropriate guidelines to inform future policy with regard to technology use with children. My director was supportive, and this feels like it may be a start to my research for this course. However, there is so much to learn, I’m not quite ready to pigeon-hole myself just yet.

Another interesting development that came out of one of those conversations with the management team was that our program coordinator pointed me in the direction of a free webinar at Early Childhood Investigations called What the Research Says About Documentation Systems & Outcome for Families, Teachers & Children, by M.E. Picher, Ph.D.. Since it’s a free webinar, and based on research in Canada, I will check it out. My only hesitation is that it is sponsored by Storypark. I’ll look a little deeper into whether the research was funded by Storypark or whether after its completion they merely funded the production of the webinar. It’ll give me a little more insight into the integrity. I’m a somewhat suspicious person, and I don’t like to take things at face value. I guess that is one of my core media literacy skills.

The webinar isn’t until September 18th, and I’ll likely watch it asynchronously as it falls during work hours, but look for a mini review in the weeks to come.

Beginning Thoughts

Children in the 21st century are avid users of technology – more so than generations past. This rise in use has led to much attention on the consequences of technology use, and how this impacts children’s brains and their socio-emotional, cognitive and physical development.  – Gottschalk, OECD, 2019

As new technologies arrive and make themselves at home in our lives, I am not alone in wondering about the impact they have on children’s development. How should we negotiate a place for these technologies in early childhood, particularly infancy?

I am interested in exploring how digital technologies impact very young children’s development and finding a balance between gaining important digital and media literacy skills and potential negative developmental outcomes. The focus of my inquiry is the age range from birth through two years.

Through the master’s of education program in Early Childhood Education at the University of Victoria, and more specifically through the Interactive and Multimedia Learning Theories course, I expect to begin my journey toward answering some of my questions about young children and technology, and most likely, finding new questions I hadn’t considered.

© 2024 Ruth's Reflections

Theme by Anders NorenUp ↑